Article
citation information:
Gądek-Hawlena, T., Michalski, K. Dangerous behavior of
pedestrians and their knowledge of road traffic risks in Poland. Scientific Journal of Silesian University of
Technology. Series Transport. 2025, 127, 57-72.
ISSN: 0209-3324. DOI: https://doi.org/10.20858/sjsutst.2025.127.4
Teresa GĄDEK-HAWLENA[1], Konrad MICHALSKI[2]
DANGEROUS BEHAVIOR
OF PEDESTRIANS AND THEIR KNOWLEDGE OF ROAD TRAFFIC RISKS IN POLAND
Summary. This article presents
the results of a survey with 3061 respondents conducted to investigate the
propensity for risky behavior among pedestrian road
users and their knowledge of the potential risks they face from passenger car
drivers. The study shows that there is a correlation between gender and risk
propensity (p=0.000, c=0.1245). Men are more likely to be risk-averse than women,
despite their greater knowledge of the possible dangers of passenger car
drivers. A similar relationship can be identified when taking into account
where pedestrians live. Residents of large cities (over 150,000 inhabitants)
show the highest propensity for risky behavior
compared to residents of smaller towns and villages. At the same time,
residents of these cities have greater knowledge than the other groups surveyed
regarding the reaction of the driver and the possibility of stopping the car.
In the case of the age of pedestrians, it is not possible to identify one
particular age group characterized by a higher propensity for risky behavior. At the same time, for the age of pedestrians and
their risky behavior, there is a significant
correlation relating to the use of headphones when crossing pedestrian
crossings without traffic lights (p=0.000, c=0.4810). Headphones were
frequently used when crossing crosswalks by those aged 18-29 years (44.2%),
while never by those aged over 65 years. The results of the study indicate the
need for educational efforts among pedestrians, taking into account demographic
variables and aspects related to limited trust in passenger car drivers.
Keywords: pedestrian, behavior, road safety, pedestrian
crossing, hazard, risk
1. INTRODUCTION
One of the problems facing the world today is road
accidents. Every day, more than 3 500 people are killed on roads around the
world, representing almost 1.3 million preventable deaths and around 50 million
injuries per year [1]. Road accidents are unpredictable events that can happen
to any road user. Although most road users are aware of the basic rules and
regulations to be observed, failure to comply with some of them leads to
disasters. Speeding, distracted drivers and pedestrians, drunk driving, and breaking
traffic laws can be cited as the main causes of this situation [2].
Many dangerous traffic situations occur involving
pedestrians. Walking is the most common form of travel. According to a study in
an Indian city, about 64.7% of journeys are made on foot, compared to 15.8% by
bicycle, 14.2% by public transport, 4.2% by private transport and only 1.1% by
other forms of transport (rickshaw, taxi) [3]. Worldwide, pedestrians account
for 23% of fatalities. In Europe, pedestrian road fatalities account for 19%
[4]. In Poland, pedestrians account for 18.3% of all road accident victims. Measures are being taken to improve
pedestrian safety, and the type and scope of these measures are included in
programs to improve road safety in Poland [5,6].
It is worth noting that pedestrians are the group of road
users who, on the one hand, are most exposed to the serious consequences of
road accidents and, on the other hand, their knowledge of safe road travel is
not verified in any way. In addition, like any group of road users, they show a
propensity for risky behavior. Risk propensity can be defined as an
individual's tendency to seek out or take risks [7]. Commonly, the term 'risky
behavior' is most often associated with the expression of a balance of gain and
loss. Psychological theories focus primarily on the assessment of loss,
indicating that a risk is any factor that has the capacity to cause an
undesirable outcome or loss of valued or expected loss (risk according to
Fishburn) [8,9]. A juxtaposition of the two groups of risk theories was made by
Zaleśkiewicz [10]. He distinguishes between two types of risk behavior: the
first one results from the desire to make a broadly understood profit or avoid
possible losses (instrumental risk), while the second refers to a specific form
of satisfying the need to experience pleasure (stimulus risk). Both of these
forms of behavior are observed among pedestrian road users [9].
One of the places where accidents involving pedestrians
most often occur is pedestrian crossings [11]. Aiming to improve safety at
pedestrian crossings without traffic lights in Poland, the law was amended in
2021 according to which: "a pedestrian at a pedestrian crossing has
priority over a vehicle. A pedestrian entering a pedestrian crossing shall have
priority over a vehicle, with the exception of a tram" [12]. As a result
of the introduced amendment broadening the rights of pedestrians, there was an
increase in the number of road accidents involving pedestrians by the end of
2022 [13]. Some of the accidents were the result of inappropriate behavior on
the part of pedestrians, who often believed that priority at pedestrian
crossings relieved them of their duty to check the traffic situation. In
addition, some pedestrians became accustomed to being a privileged group of
road users at pedestrian crossings and therefore stopped being cautious by
crossing without checking for oncoming vehicles, regardless of the prevailing
weather conditions and time of day.
The article presents the results of research, following
the 2021 amendments to the Traffic Law, on:
-
risky
behavior among pedestrian road users in Poland, mainly at pedestrian crossings,
-
knowledge
of the risks to which pedestrians are exposed by car drivers resulting from the
sudden appearance of a pedestrian on the road and the driver's reaction time
and ability to stop the vehicle.
2. LITERATURE
REVIEW
Another
aspect that should be considered by pedestrians when crossing a pedestrian
crossing without traffic lights is the awareness of the driver's reaction time
to the appearance of an obstacle. This is assumed to be between 0.7 and 1
second on average [14]. At the same time, according to studies, the driver's
reaction time to the appearance of an obstacle can vary according to gender,
age, speed, distance, lighting, attention focus, fatigue, driver experience,
seat position, brake light location, or mobile phone use [15-17]. Another
aspect that should be considered by pedestrians when crossing a crosswalk
without traffic lights is awareness of the driver's reaction time to the
appearance of an obstacle. It is generally accepted that it averages 0.7 to 1
second [14]. At the same time, according to studies, the driver's reaction time
to the appearance of an obstacle can vary depending on gender, age, speed,
distance, lighting, concentration of attention, fatigue, driver experience,
seat position, location of brake lights, or use of a cell phone [15,17].
The
driver's reaction time is one factor affecting accident-free driving. The
second factor, in the case of a pedestrian suddenly appearing on the road, is
the braking distance of the vehicle. The distance the vehicle travels when
perceiving and reacting to a situation and reacting to a dangerous situation is
proportional to the reaction time. This time accounts for a large proportion
(25-50%) of the total stopping distance. The longer the reaction time, the
greater the stopping distance. Braking distance is a function of human and
mechanical processes related to perception, reaction and stopping time [18,19].
Stopping distances, according to research, depend on a number of factors,
driver-dependent and non-driver-dependent. However, studies show that weather
and lighting conditions, such as snow, rain, fog, high winds and night and day
driving, affect braking distance by affecting vehicle performance, traffic flow
and driver visibility [18,20-22]. This means that suddenly stepping into a
pedestrian crossing in front of a moving vehicle, or crossing the road in an
unauthorized place in adverse weather conditions or poor lighting, makes an
accident more likely.
Braking
speed is significantly influenced by visibility. It is, in the case of the
pedestrian, largely dependent on it. According to a study by Doza et al. [23],
the response process of drivers is highly dependent on the relationship between
the time a pedestrian becomes visible and the speed of the pedestrian,
suggesting that the response of drivers may depend more on the pedestrian's
field of safety than on themselves. Most people travelling at night do not
realize how much visibility a driver has after dark. If road conditions force
the driver to travel with dipped headlights on, the headlights can illuminate
the road in front of the vehicle for as little as 40m. A pedestrian, on the
other hand, is visible from an even shorter distance, as the light beam must
illuminate him or her for approximately 25-30 cm (measured from the road
surface) to be visible to the driver. This essentially means that the distance
to spot a pedestrian is much shorter (approximately 20-25 m) [24,25]. According
to a study by Benea et al. [26], pedestrian clothing and adaptive headlights
significantly affect the likelihood that a driver will recognize the presence
of a pedestrian on the road and correctly perceive the distance the pedestrian
is at. Pedestrians' clothing has a huge impact on their visibility. When the
pedestrian was dressed in a black coat, he or she was barely noticed from a
distance of 10 m. For the other subjects, the distance of the pedestrian in
black was confused with the background (i.e. it was invisible). The moment of
visibility of a pedestrian wearing black may prevent drivers from stopping in
time. Pedestrian visibility is improved by wearing light-colored
clothing (which provides greater contrast with the surroundings) [25,27,28].
A
study by Gauld et al. [29] found that pedestrians who were dependent on their
mobile phones were more likely to use them when crossing the road. Teenagers in
particular have become dependent on their phones, which affects their decisions
to cross the carriageway, increasing the likelihood of a collision or accident.
In addition to chatting, texting or using various chat rooms is dangerous when
crossing the road. This influences slower walking speed and holding the head in
a bent position to view the device screen while walking [30-33]. People who are
busy with their devices are more likely to cross the road in front of oncoming
traffic [34,35,30] Equally dangerous is the use of headphones. A study by Lee
et al. [36] shows that pedestrians are actually unable to detect the warning sound
of a vehicle coming from behind, even from a short distance (1 m) if they are
listening to music from headphones at the time. Wearing headphones is
associated with an increase in walking speed [37]. According to a number of studies,
both mobile phone and headphone use by pedestrians contributes to reduced road
safety [38-41].
The
factors outlined as potential pedestrian safety hazards also depend on the
pedestrian's age [42-45], gender [44-48], or place of residence [49-51].
3. MATERIAL
AND METHODS
The
study used a survey questionnaire. It was designed using Microsoft Forms tools
and was distributed via email and social media channels; Facebook, Linkedln and Instagram. A pilot study was conducted to test
the readability of the worded questions and responses.
The
questionnaire consisted of questions about the respondents' unsafe behavior and their knowledge of selected car driver behaviors that could pose a significant risk to
pedestrians. The questions related to:
̶
the types
of crossings most frequently used by pedestrians and the frequency of use of
crossings without traffic lights,
̶
dangerous
behavior of pedestrians including crossing the road in an unauthorized place,
use of telephones and headphones when crossing a pedestrian crossing without
traffic lights, use of reflective elements after dark,
̶
awareness
of pedestrians regarding their visibility after dark without the use of
reflective elements, awareness of the reaction time of a driver to the sudden
appearance of an obstacle or a living person on the road, knowledge of the
braking distance of a passenger vehicle at 50 km/h, knowledge of the distance
covered by a passenger car travelling at 50 km/h in 1 s,
̶
demographic
data (gender, age, education, place of residence).
The
survey was conducted between 20.12.2022 and 01.06.2023. Two methods were used
in the data collection process:
̶
PAPI - to
collect data among a proportion of people over 65 who do not use social media
(e.g. due to lack of internet access) and a proportion of rural residents who
had no other means of completing the survey;
̶
CAWI - to
collect data from the remaining respondents.
In
determining the sample size, a confidence level of α =95%, p= 0.5
and a maximum error of 2% were assumed. The minimum sample size for this survey
was N=2401. In total, responses were obtained from 3251 respondents. 190 paper
questionnaires were rejected due to incorrect completion. 3061 questionnaires
were used for further analysis, representing 94.2% of all completed
questionnaires. In order to answer the research questions, statistical analyses
were carried out using an Excel spreadsheet and the Statistica 13.3 program,
which was used to calculate basic descriptive statistics, the χ2 test and
Pearson's c- contingency coefficient. The significance level in the article was
taken as α =
0.05.
4. RESULTS
4.1.
Participant
The
characteristics of the respondents are summarized in Table 1. More than half of
the respondents were women - 1,840 people (60.1%). The largest group were
respondents aged 18-29 years - 889 people (29.0%). Most of the respondents had
higher education - 1622 persons (53.0%) and secondary education - 978 persons
(32.0%), and the least with vocational education - 168 persons (5.5%). In terms
of occupational status, most respondents were employed - 1,765 persons (57.7%),
while the least were unemployed - 67 persons (2.2%). Among the respondents,
people residing in cities predominated - 1,883 persons (59.9%), including those
in urban centers with more than 150,000 inhabitants -
870 persons (28.4%).
Tab. 1
Characteristics of respondents
Specification |
Number |
% |
|
gender |
woman |
1840 |
60,1 |
man |
1221 |
39,9 |
|
age |
under 18* |
268 |
8,8 |
18-29 |
889 |
29,0 |
|
30-41 |
526 |
17,2 |
|
42-53 |
692 |
22,6 |
|
54-65 |
329 |
10,7 |
|
over 65 |
357 |
11,7 |
|
education |
primary |
293 |
9,6 |
vocational |
168 |
5,5 |
|
secondary |
978 |
32,0 |
|
higher |
1622 |
53,0 |
|
residence |
village |
1228 |
40,1 |
city of up to 50 thousand
inhabitants |
564 |
18,4 |
|
city from 51 thousand - 100
thousand inhabitants |
289 |
9,4 |
|
city from 101 thousand - 150
thousand inhabitants |
110 |
3,6 |
|
above 150,000 inhabitants |
870 |
28,4 |
*the survey
included people who were 14 years of age or older (persons who have completed
elementary school).
4.2. Dangerous
behavior and risk awareness - by gender
Considering the unsafe behavior among men and
women, it can be seen that men were more likely to exhibit unsafe behavior than women, but this is not a significant
difference (Tab. 2). Men were more likely to cross the road in an unauthorized
place (79.0% of men and 67.6% of women). Men were also more likely to use
phones and headphones when crossing a pedestrian crossing without a traffic
light. When it came to the use of reflective elements after dark, men were also
more likely to indicate that they did not use them at all. In the area of
unsafe behavior, there was a weak correlation only
between gender and crossing the road in an unauthorized place (p=0.000,
c=0.1245). Although men were more likely to exhibit unsafe behavior,
they have greater knowledge of the dangers of car drivers. Considering the
knowledge of the distance from which a pedestrian walking at dusk without
reflective elements is visible, a weak correlation relationship can be
indicated (p=0.000, c = 0.1798). Similar results and a weak correlation
relationship were also obtained for:
̶
knowledge of the driver's reaction time to the sudden
appearance of an obstacle or living creature on the road (p=0.000, c = 0.2627),
̶
knowledge of the braking distance of a passenger car at 50
km/h (p=0.000, c = 0.1548),
̶
knowledge of the distance covered by a passenger car in
one second travelling at 50 km/h (p=0.000, c = 0.1851).
Tab. 2
Dangerous behavior
and risk awareness
Gender |
Woman |
Men |
Test results |
c-Pearson coefficient |
|||
Number |
% |
Number |
% |
||||
Crossing the
road in a forbidden place |
yes |
1243 |
67,6 |
965 |
79,0 |
χ2=48,114 p=0,000 |
c = 0,1245 |
no |
597 |
32,4 |
256 |
21,0 |
|||
Crossing a
pedestrian crossing without a traffic light using a cell phone |
never |
1059 |
57,6 |
655 |
53,6 |
χ2 = 8,597 p=0,0136 |
c = 0,0529 |
rarely |
635 |
34,5 |
435 |
35,7 |
|||
often |
146 |
7,9 |
131 |
10,7 |
|||
Crossing a
crosswalk without a traffic light using headphones |
never |
1158 |
62,9 |
679 |
55,6 |
χ2=17,180 p=0,000 |
c = 0,0747 |
rarely |
319 |
17,3 |
267 |
21,9 |
|||
often |
363 |
19,7 |
275 |
22,5 |
|||
Use of reflective
elements after darkness falls |
yes |
368 |
20,0 |
197 |
16,1 |
χ2 =7,760 p =0,021 |
c = 0,0503 |
sometimes |
686 |
37,3 |
463 |
37,9 |
|||
no |
786 |
42,7 |
561 |
46,0 |
|||
Knowledge of
the distance from which a pedestrian walking at dusk without reflective
elements is visible |
good |
232 |
12,6 |
319 |
26,1 |
χ2=102,299 p =0,000 |
c = 0,1798 |
wrong |
1410 |
76,6 |
831 |
68,1 |
|||
no opinion |
198 |
10,8 |
71 |
5,8 |
Knowledge of
the driver's reaction time to the sudden appearance of an obstacle or living
being on the road |
good |
433 |
23,5 |
599 |
49,1 |
χ2=226,848 p =0,000 |
c = 0,2627 |
wrong |
1157 |
62,9 |
553 |
45,3 |
|||
no opinion |
250 |
13,6 |
69 |
5,7 |
|||
Knowledge of
the braking distance of a passenger vehicle at 50 km/h |
good |
427 |
23,2 |
401 |
32,8 |
χ2=75,899 p =0,000 |
c = 0,1548 |
wrong |
1118 |
60,8 |
737 |
60,4 |
|||
no opinion |
295 |
16,0 |
83 |
6,8 |
|||
Knowledge of
the distance covered by a passenger car in one second traveling at 50 km/h |
good |
685 |
37,2 |
654 |
53,6 |
χ2=108,539 p =0,000 |
c = 0,1851 |
wrong |
763 |
41,5 |
449 |
36,8 |
|||
no opinion |
392 |
21,3 |
118 |
9,7 |
4.3
Dangerous behavior and risk awareness - by age
When
analyzing the dangerous behavior
of pedestrians and their knowledge of traffic hazards, it is possible to
indicate the existence of a correlation relationship for each study area (Tab.
3). People between the ages of 18 and 29 were most likely to cross the road in
an unauthorized place (80.5%), and those over 65 were least likely to do so
(57.1%). There is a weak correlation between age and crossing the road in a
prohibited place (p=0.000, c=0.1687). At the same time, it can be noted that
although under-18s are most likely not to cross in a prohibited place, they
show dangerous behavior at crossings without traffic
lights, using mobile phones and headphones.
The largest proportion of under-18s, among all respondents considering
the age criterion, frequently use mobile phones (19.8%), as do those aged 18 -
29 (15.1%). The largest group of people who never use mobile phones when
crossing a pedestrian crossing are those aged 54-65 (79.0%) and those over 65
(79%). There is a moderate correlation between the variables (p=0.000,
c=0.3057).
There
is a similar trend for headphones. Those aged under 18 (32.2%) and 18 - 29
(44.2%) use headphones frequently when crossing crossings without traffic
lights. The highest proportion of pedestrians who do not use headphones when
crossing pedestrian crossings without traffic lights includes those aged 54 -
65 (91.5%) and over 65 (95.2%). There is a strong correlation between age and
crossing pedestrian crossings without traffic lights (p=0.000, c = 0.4810).
The
situation is slightly different when it comes to using reflective elements
after dark. The elderly (over 65 - 51.5 %) are the ones who do not wear such
items. In each of the groups surveyed, less than 25% wear reflective elements.
Those aged under 18 (41.4%) and 30-41 (41.4%) were the most likely to indicate
these elements as being worn 'sometimes'.
Analyzing the knowledge
of pedestrians about possible dangerous situations related to road traffic and
their dangerous behavior, it can be seen that more
than half of the respondents did not know the correct answer to most of the
questions. In the case of knowledge of the distance from which a pedestrian is
visible when walking at dusk without reflective elements, the most common
correct answer was indicated by those aged 54 - 65 (22.5%), those over 65
(22.4%) and those under 18 (21.6%). When analyzing
the question concerning the wearing of reflective elements after darkness
falls, it can be seen that the people who most often knew the positive answer
were the least likely to wear such elements. The most frequent correct answers
to the question on knowledge of the reaction time of a driver to the sudden
appearance of an obstacle or living creature on the road were given by people
aged 18 - 29 (36.7%) and 42 - 53 (37.3%). At the same time, one of the higher
percentages of incorrect answers was recorded in both groups. When asked about
the braking distance of a vehicle travelling at 50 km/h, the most common
incorrect answers were given by those aged 18 - 29 (66.3%) and 30 - 41 (61.2%).
Incorrect answers were indicated least frequently by people over 65 years of
age (51.0%), while at the same time marking the answer "I have no
opinion" most frequently (20.2%). There is a weak correlation between the
variables (p=0.000, c = 0.2234). In the case of the question about the distance
covered by a passenger car in 1 s, travelling at 50 km/h, the most frequent
incorrect answer was given by those aged 30 - 41 years (43.7%) and the correct
one by those aged 18 - 29 years (49.3%).
Tab. 3
Dangerous behavior
and risk awareness - by age
age |
under 18 |
18-29 |
30-41 |
42-53 |
54-65 |
over 65 |
test results |
c-Pearson coefficient |
|
% |
% |
% |
% |
% |
% |
||||
Crossing the road in a forbidden place |
yes |
69,0 |
80,5 |
77,6 |
69,1 |
66,0 |
57,1 |
χ2=89,636 p=0,000 |
c=0,1687 |
no |
31,0 |
19,5 |
22,4 |
30,9 |
34,0 |
42,9 |
|||
Crossing a pedestrian crossing without a traffic
light using a cell phone |
never |
39,6 |
42,0 |
50,6 |
61,7 |
79,0 |
79,0 |
χ2=315,508 p=0,000 |
c=0,3057 |
rarely |
40,7 |
42,9 |
40,9 |
34,0 |
19,1 |
18,8 |
|||
often |
19,8 |
15,1 |
8,5 |
4,3 |
1,8 |
2,2 |
|||
Crossing a crosswalk without a traffic light using
headphones |
never |
32,1 |
29,2 |
61,8 |
75,9 |
91,5 |
95,2 |
χ2=921,807 p=0,000 |
c=0,4810 |
rarely |
28,7 |
26,6 |
24,3 |
16,6 |
6,4 |
2,5 |
|||
often |
39,2 |
44,2 |
13,9 |
7,5 |
2,1 |
2,2 |
|||
Use of reflective elements after darkness falls |
never |
13,1 |
13,2 |
20,3 |
22,8 |
24,6 |
18,8 |
χ2=61,690 p= 0,000 |
c=0,1406 |
rarely |
41,4 |
37,7 |
41,4 |
38,3 |
34,7 |
29,7 |
|||
often |
45,5 |
49,2 |
38,2 |
38,9 |
40,7 |
51,5 |
|||
Knowledge of the distance from which a pedestrian
walking at dusk without reflective elements is visible |
good |
21,6 |
17,8 |
13,9 |
15,6 |
22,5 |
22,4 |
χ2=172,591 p=0,000 |
c=0,2310 |
wrong |
54,5 |
76,7 |
82,5 |
77,5 |
68,7 |
60,8 |
|||
no opinion |
23,9 |
5,5 |
3,6 |
6,9 |
8,8 |
16,8 |
|||
Knowledge of the driver's reaction time to the
sudden appearance of an obstacle or living being on the road |
good |
29,9 |
36,7 |
34,0 |
37,3 |
34,7 |
21,0 |
χ2=130,361 p=0,000 |
c=0,2021 |
wrong |
50,7 |
58,5 |
58,9 |
53,8 |
50,5 |
57,7 |
|||
no opinion |
19,4 |
4,8 |
7,0 |
9,0 |
14,9 |
21,3 |
Knowledge of the braking distance of a passenger
vehicle at 50 km/h |
good |
12,7 |
27,4 |
30,8 |
29,2 |
25,2 |
28,9 |
χ2=160,809 p=0,000 |
c=0,2234 |
wrong |
57,8 |
66,3 |
61,2 |
60,4 |
57,5 |
51,0 |
|||
no opinion |
29,5 |
6,3 |
8,0 |
10,4 |
17,3 |
20,2 |
|||
Knowledge of the distance covered by a passenger car
in one second traveling at 50 km/h |
good |
28,4 |
49,3 |
43,7 |
43,6 |
41,0 |
44,3 |
χ2=94,121 p=0,000 |
c=0,1727 |
wrong |
41,0 |
38,9 |
43,7 |
41,2 |
38,9 |
31,7 |
|||
no opinion |
30,6 |
11,8 |
12,5 |
15,2 |
20,1 |
24,1 |
4.4 Dangerous behavior and risk awareness - by residence
The
place of residence and unsafe behavior and knowledge
of traffic hazards is very important due to the different types of roads that
run through rural and urban areas. The results of the survey indicate that
pedestrians living in rural areas (76.7%) and residents of cities with up to
50,000 inhabitants (72.0%) are most likely to cross the road in unauthorized
places (Tab. 4). In each case, the percentage of responses indicating
negative behavior was over 60%. There is a weak
correlation between the variables (p=0.000, c=0.1126). Pedestrians living in
large cities (more than 150,000 inhabitants) are more likely to use a mobile
phone and headphones when crossing a crosswalk without traffic lights. The
correlation relationship between the variables is very weak for mobile phone
use (p=0.000, c = 0.1114) and weak for headphone use (p=0.000, c=0.2163).
In
the case of wearing reflective elements after dark, respondents' answers were
specific to their place of residence. Pedestrians living in rural areas are
most likely to wear reflective elements - 24.3% of indications
"always", and residents of cities with more than 150,000 inhabitants
were least likely to wear reflective elements - 12.8% of indications
"always". At the same time, pedestrians living in the countryside
most often indicated the wrong answer regarding the distance from which a
pedestrian moving after dusk without reflective elements is visible.
Wearing
reflective elements in the countryside is defined by law. In undeveloped areas,
pedestrians are obliged to wear them. This requirement does not correspond to
the respondents' knowledge of reflective elements. In both the question on
pedestrian visibility and the other questions, the percentage of incorrect
answers is more than 50%. Residents of cities with more than 150,000
inhabitants (36.5%) indicated the highest number of correct answers regarding
the reaction time of a driver to the sudden appearance of an obstacle or living
being on the road, while the lowest number of correct answers was indicated by
pedestrians living in cities with 101,000 to 150,000 inhabitants (24.5%).
Residents of cities with 101,000 to 150,000 inhabitants also indicated the
incorrect answer most often (65.5%). Also, for the other questions, residents
of cities with more than 150,000 inhabitants most often indicated a positive
answer.
In
the case of the question concerning knowledge of the braking distance of a
passenger car at 50 km/h, the correct answer was given by 30.6% of pedestrians
from cities with a population of 150,000, and in the case of the question
concerning knowledge of the distance covered by a passenger car in 1 second
travelling at 50 km/h, the correct answer was given by 48.9% of residents of
the largest cities in Poland. Taking into account the answers of pedestrians
living in rural areas, where sometimes moving on the road requires more
attention from them, the percentage of incorrect answers is higher for most
questions than for pedestrians living in cities.
Tab. 4
Dangerous behavior
and risk awareness - by residence
Residence
|
Village |
city of up
to 50 thou. inhabitants |
city from 51
thou.- 100 thou. inhabitants |
city from
101 thou. - 150 thou. inhabitants |
above 150,000 inhabitants |
test results |
c-Pearson coefficient |
|
% |
% |
% |
% |
% |
||||
Crossing the road in a forbidden place |
yes |
76,7 |
72,0 |
60,2 |
63,6 |
70,8 |
χ2=39,294 p=0,000 |
c=0,1126 |
no |
23,3 |
28,0 |
39,8 |
36,4 |
29,2 |
|||
Crossing a pedestrian crossing without a traffic
light using a cell phone |
never |
60,8 |
54,8 |
60,9 |
55,3 |
48,4 |
χ2=38,474 p=0,000 |
c=0,1114 |
rarely |
31,4 |
35,1 |
32,9 |
37,7 |
40,3 |
|||
often |
7,8 |
10,1 |
6,2 |
7,0 |
11,3 |
|||
Crossing a crosswalk without a traffic light using
headphones |
never |
69,5 |
58,9 |
65,1 |
67,3 |
45,0 |
χ2=150,249 p=0,000 |
c=0,2163 |
rarely |
17,3 |
19,1 |
16,6 |
14,5 |
23,0 |
|||
often |
13,2 |
22,0 |
18,3 |
12,7 |
32,0 |
|||
Use of reflective elements after darkness falls |
never |
24,3 |
16,5 |
14,5 |
18,2 |
12,8 |
χ2=100,876 p=0,000 |
c=0,1812 |
rarely |
41,5 |
33,9 |
30,1 |
32,7 |
37,4 |
|||
often |
34,1 |
49,6 |
55,4 |
49,1 |
49,9 |
|||
Knowledge of the distance from which a pedestrian
walking at dusk without reflective elements is visible |
good |
16,7 |
16,5 |
19,7 |
18,2 |
20,2 |
χ2=20,280 p=0,000 |
c=0,0811 |
wrong |
76,4 |
72,2 |
68,5 |
71,8 |
71,1 |
|||
no opinion |
6,9 |
11,3 |
11,8 |
10,0 |
8,6 |
|||
Knowledge of the driver's reaction time to the sudden
appearance of an obstacle or living being on the road |
good |
32,7 |
35,1 |
32,2 |
24,5 |
36,0 |
χ2=13,945 p=0,083 |
- |
wrong |
57,7 |
52,1 |
55,0 |
65,5 |
54,8 |
|||
no opinion |
9,7 |
12,8 |
12,8 |
10,0 |
9,2 |
|||
Knowledge of the braking distance of a passenger
vehicle at 50 km/h |
good |
26,2 |
25,9 |
21,5 |
29,1 |
30,6 |
χ2=37,025 p=0,000 |
c=0,1093 |
wrong |
62,8 |
56,0 |
62,6 |
60,0 |
59,9 |
|||
no opinion |
11,0 |
18,1 |
15,9 |
10,9 |
9,5 |
|||
Knowledge of the distance covered by a passenger car
in one second traveling at 50 km/h |
good |
42,7 |
40,8 |
39,4 |
41,8 |
48,9 |
χ2=21,686 p=0,006 |
c=0,0839 |
wrong |
40,1 |
41,1 |
39,1 |
39,1 |
38,2 |
|||
no opinion |
17,3 |
18,1 |
21,5 |
19,1 |
13,0 |
5. DISCUSSION
With
reference to the results of the analyses presented, it can be seen that the
majority of pedestrians are prone to jaywalking. Taking into account the
demographic variable of gender, it can be indicated that men were more likely
to be prone to crossing in a prohibited place than women. The findings are
confirmed by studies [47,44,52]. Those aged 18 - 29 years had the highest
propensity to cross in a forbidden place, while those aged over 65 years had
the lowest propensity. For those over 65 years of age, this behavior
is influenced by their psychophysical fitness or lower propensity to engage in
risky behavior [43,44,45]. Considering the place of
residence, despite slight differences, the predominant group among respondents
prone to dangerous behavior were rural residents.
This behavior may be a result of the low number of
marked pedestrian crossings found in rural areas.
Another
risky behavior among pedestrians is the use of mobile
phones and headphones at pedestrian crossings without traffic lights. Although
there is no significant difference in the number of responses, men are more
likely to use phones and headphones at crossings than women. They are mainly
young and living in the largest cities with more than 150,000 inhabitants. Some
of the observations are consistent with previous ones [41,38,39].
The
final type of unsafe behavior among pedestrians that
was the focus of the study was the use of reflective elements by pedestrians
after dark. When comparing the gender data, it can be indicated that, despite a
slight difference, it is more often women who wear these types of elements than
men. Taking into account the age criterion, it was mainly people aged 54-64,
while the least frequent were those aged over 65. Pedestrians living in rural
areas are most likely to use reflective elements after dark, while those living
in cities with more than 150 000 inhabitants are least likely to do so. This
distribution of responses may be due to two reasons. Rural residents, are more
likely to walk in an undeveloped area, where it is legally obligatory to wear
reflective elements. For residents of large cities, the use of reflectors is
not necessarily necessary, as most pedestrian crossings or streets are well
lit.
At
the same time, the majority of pedestrians are not aware of the distance from
which a pedestrian moving after dark is visible. In each of the surveyed groups
(taking into account the criteria of gender, age and place of residence), the
percentage of incorrect answers was more than 54%.
From
the analysis of the respondents' answers concerning their knowledge of the
driver's reaction time and the possibility of stopping the vehicle, it can be
clearly seen that knowledge among pedestrians is very low, which is certainly
reflected in the number of accidents. At the same time, some correlations can
be identified. Considering the gender of pedestrians, men are more
knowledgeable about driver behavior than women, while
at the same time they show a greater tendency towards dangerous behavior than women. In the case of the age of pedestrians
and their knowledge of drivers' reaction times and braking distances, it is not
possible to distinguish a single group giving mostly correct answers. What is
apparent, however, is the correlation between place of residence and the
correct answers given. Inhabitants of cities with more than 150,000 inhabitants
gave the most correct answers.
The
research carried out clearly shows that the vast majority of pedestrians
surveyed have very low knowledge and awareness of the dangers of inappropriate
road behavior and, on the other hand, a significant
propensity to take risky actions. The lack of awareness concerning, among other
things, the braking distance of a vehicle among pedestrians certainly has a
negative impact on their safety. The above findings correspond with those of Olakulehin et al. [53] and Jothula
& Sreeharshika [54].
6.
RECOMMENDATIONS AND LIMITATIONS
The
conducted research provides a lot of valuable knowledge about the behavior of pedestrian road users and indicates the need
for their education. This research can be used to develop educational programs
taking into account age, gender and place of residence. An important element of
such programs should be raising awareness of the threats posed by drivers of
motor vehicles, which are often influenced by pedestrians themselves.
However,
these studies have certain limitations. Their significant limitation was
conducting research in two forms: online and paper. It seems advisable to
undertake further research on the knowledge of pedestrian road users, extending
it to include legal aspects regarding safe road travel and identifying tools
that would influence their behavior.
References
1.
UN. 2020.
“Resolution adopted by the General Assembly on 31 August 2020”. A/RES/74/299.
2.
Mukherjee Deotima, Primat Saha, 2022. “Walking behaviour
and safety of pedestrians at different types of facilities: a review of recent
research and future research needs”. SN Social Sciences 2: 76. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s43545-022-00384-x.
3.
Arasan V.
Thamizh, Rengaraju V. Raghu, Rao K.V. Kriszna. 1994. „Characteristics of trip by foot and bicycle mode in an
Indian city”. Journal of
Transportation Engeneering 120(2): 283-294. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1061/(ASCE)0733-947X(1994)120:2(283).
4.
WHO.
2023. Global status report on road safety 2023. Available at: http://iris.who.int/bitstream/handle/10665/375016/9789240086517-eng.pdf?sequence=1.
5.
Ministerstwo
Infrastruktury. 2020. Narodowy Program Bezpieczeństwa Ruchu Drogowego
2020-2030. Available at: https://www.krbrd.gov.pl/narodowy-program-brd-2021-2030. [In Polish: Ministry of Infrastructure. 2020. National
Road Safety Program 2020-2030].
6.
Rada Ministrów. 2021. Program
bezpiecznej infrastruktury drogowej. Available at: Treść_Programu_Bezpiecznej_Infrastruktury_Drogowej_na_lata_2021-2024.pdf. [In Polish: Council of Ministers. 2021. Safe road
infrastructure program].
7.
Studenski Ryszard. 2004. Ryzyko i ryzykowanie. Katowice:
Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego. ISBN: 978-8-3226-1394-8.
[In Polish: Risks and taking chances. Katowice: University of Silesia Publishing House].
8.
Goszczyńska
Maryla, Ryszard Studenski. 2006. Psychologia zachowań ryzykownych. Koncepcje, badania, praktyka.
Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Akademickie ŻAK. [In Polish: The psychology of risk behavior.
Concepts, research, practice. Warsaw: Academic Publishing House ŻAK].
9.
Cybulski
Marcin. 2008. „Bezpieczeństwo ruchu drogowego (BRD) a psychologiczne podstawy zachowań ryzykownych na drogach”. In: Bartkowiak G. Czynniki
kształtujące zachowania zdrowotne człowieka na przestrzeni życia. Poznań: Wydawnictwo UM w Poznaniu. [In Polish: “Road traffic safety (BRD)
and the psychological basis of risk behaviors on the roads”. In: Bartkowiak G. Factors shaping human
health behaviors over the life span. Poznan: UM Publishing House in
Poznan].
10.
Zaleśkiewicz Tomasz. 2008.
„Samoregulacja i podejmowanie ryzyka. Rola procesów automatycznych”. [In Polish:
„Self-regulation and risk taking: The role of automatic processes”]. Czasopismo Psychologiczne 14(2): 287-296.
11.
Brudzinski Marcin, Anna Gobis, Lucyna Guminska,
Lukasz Jelinski, Mariusz Kiec,
Piotr Tomczuk. 2021. “Assessment of
the Influence of Road Infrastructure Parameters on the Behaviour of Drivers and
Pedestrians in Pedestrian Crossing Areas”. Energies 14: 3559. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/ en14123559.
12.
Ustawa z dnia 20 czerwca
1997 r. Prawo o ruchu drogowym. Dz.U.
1997, nr 99, poz. 602. [In Polish: Law of June 20, 1997 - Law on Road Traffic.
Journal of Laws. 1997, no. 99, item 602].
13.
Komenda Główna Policji.
2023. Wypadki drogowe w Polsce. Available
at: https://statystyka.policja.pl/st/ruch-drogowy/76562,Wypadki-drogowe-raporty-roczne.html. [In Polish: Police Headquarters. 2023. Road
accidents in Poland].
14.
Draguła Sławomir. 2013. Droga hamowania to nie wszystko - ile
miejsca potrzeba, by zatrzymać auto. Available
at: https://motofakty.pl/droga-hamowania-to-nie-wszystko-ile-miejsca-potrzeba-by-zatrzymac-auto/ar/c4-16232753. [In Polish: Draguła Sławomir. 2013. Stopping distance
is not everything - how much space is needed to stop the car.].
15.
Stańczyk
Tomasz, Rafał Jurecki, Zbigniew Lozia, Wiesław
Pieniążek. 2011. „Wpływ wieku i doświadczenia kierowców na uzyskiwane wartości
czasów reakcji”. Paragraf Na Drodze. Prawne i kryminalistyczne problemy
ruchu drogowego. Numer specjalny – październik: 339-352. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Instytutu Ekspertyz
Sądowych w Krakowie. ISSN: 1505-3520. [In Polish: “Influence of age and
experience of drivers on the obtained values of reaction times”. Paragraph
on the Road. Legal and Forensic Problems of Road Traffic. Special issue - October: 339-352. Cracow:
Publishing House of the Institute of Forensic Expertise in Cracow].
16.
Kolekar Servesh, Joost Winter, David Abbink. 2020. “Which
parts of the road guide obstacle avoidance?”. Quantifying the driver's risk
field. Applied ergonomics 89: 103196. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2020.103196.
17.
Chen Weiyo, Tetsho Sawaragi, Toshihiro Hiraoka. 2023.
“Comparing driver reaction and mental workload of visual and auditory take-over
request from perspective of driver characteristics and eye-tracking metrics”. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic
Psychology and Behaviour 97: 396-410. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2023.07.012.
18.
Zurlinden Hendrik, John Gaffney, Elizabeth Hovenden. 2023.
“Can I Stop? Considering Opportunities to Influence Multidisciplinary Factors
That Result in Crashes”. Journal of Road Safety 34(3): 57-69. DOI: https://doi.org/10.33492/JRS-D-22/00008.
19.
Droździel Paweł, Sławomir Tarkowski,
Iwona Rybicka, Rafał Wrona. 2020. „Drivers ’reaction time research in the conditions in
the real traffic”. Open Engineering 10(1): 35-47. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/eng-2020-0004.
20.
Singh Harpreet, Ankit Kathuria. 2021. “Analyzing driver
behavior under naturalistic driving conditions: A review”. Accident
Analysis & Prevention 150: 105908. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105908.
21.
Druta Cristian, Andriew Kassing, Roland Gibbons, Andrew Alden
Va. 2020. “Assessing driver behavior using shrp2 adverse weather data”. Journal of Safety Research 73: 283-295. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2020.03.013.
22.
Samson Christian, Qinaot Hussain, Wael Alhajyaseen, 2022.
“Analysis of stopping sight distance (SSD) parameters: a review study”. Procedia Computer Science 201: 126-133. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2022.03.019.
23.
Dozza Marco, Christian-Nils Boda, Leila Jaber, Thalya
Prateek, Nils Lubbe. 2020. “How do drivers negotiate intersections with
pedestrians? The importance of pedestrian time-to-arrival and visibility”. Accident Analysis & Prevention 141: 105524. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2020.105524.
24.
Kępa
Paweł. 2020. “The issue of road visibility and road traffic safety”. Humanities
and Social Sciences 27(4): 39-49.
25.
Babić Darko, Dario Babić, Mario Fiolić, Marija Ferko. 2021. „Factors affecting pedestrian conspicuity at night:
Analysis based on driver eye tracking”. Safety Science 139: 105257.
26.
Benea Bogdan, Daniel Trusca, George Toganel, Alexandru Radu.
2019. “Pedestrian visibility at night: the influence of the pedestrian clothing
and the defective headlamp of the car”. IOP Conference Series: Materials
Science and Engineering 568(1): 012003. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1088/1757-899X/568/1/012003.
27.
González-Gómez Keila, Maria Castro. 2019. „Evaluating
pedestrians’ safety on Urban intersections: A visibility analysis”. Sustainability 11(23): 6630. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/su11236630.
28.
Park Yunmi, Max Garcia. 2020. “Pedestrian safety perception
and urban street settings”. International Journal of Sustainable
Transportation 14(11): 860-871. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15568318.2019.1641577.
29.
Gauld Cassandrea, Ioni Lewis, Katherine White. 2014. “Concealing their communication:
Exploring psychosocial predictors of young drivers’ intentions and engagement
in concealed texting”. Accident Analysis & Prevention 62: 285-293. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2013.10.016.
30. Pelicioni Paulo, Lioyd Chan, Shvotong Shi,
Lauren Kark, Yoshiro Okubo, Matthew Brodie. 2023. “Impact of mobile phone use on accidental falls
risk in young adult pedestrians”. Heliyon 9(8). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e18366.
31.
BakhtariAghdam Fatemeb, Samaneh Aliasgharzadeh, Homayoun Sadeghi-Bazargani,
Sepiden Harzand-Jadidi. 2023. “Pedestrians’ unsafe road-crossing behaviors in
Iran: An observational-based study in West
Azerbaijan”. Traffic injury prevention 24(7): 638-644. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15389588.2023.2237152.
32.
Wachnicka
Joanna, Karol Kulesza. 2020. “Does the Use of Cell Phones and Headphones at the
Signalised Pedestrian Crossings Increse the Risk of Accident?”. The Baltic
Journal of Road and Bridge Engineering 15: 96-108. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7250/bjrbe.2020-15.496.
33.
Mikušová
Miroslava., Joanna Wachnick, Joanna Zukowska. 2021. „Research on the Use of
Mobile Devices and Headphones on Pedestrian Crossings – Pilot Case Study from Slovakia”. Safety 7:17. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/safety7010017.
34.
Glassbrennen
Donna, Tony Ye. 2007. Driver Cell Phone Use in 2006 – Overall Results.
Available at: http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/Pubs/810790.pdf.
35.
Nasar Jack, Derek Troyer. 2013. “Pedestrian injuries due to
mobile phone use in public places”. Accident Analysis & Prevention 57: 91-95. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2013.03.021.
36.
Lee Hsiao, Ziyu Bai, Yenn. S. Ho, Jun Soh, Heow Lee. 2020.
“Effect of music from headphone on pedestrians”. Applied acoustics 169: 107485. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apacoust.2020.107485.
37.
Mohammed Hammed. 2021. “Assessment of distracted pedestrian
crossing behavior at midblock crosswalks”. IATSS Research 45(4): 584-593. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.iatssr.2021.07.003.
38.
Hatfield Julie, Susanne Murphy. 2007. “The effects of mobile
phone use on pedestrian crossing behaviour at signalised and unsignalized
intersections”. Accident Analysis & Prevention 39(1): 197-205. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2006.07.001.
39. Lichenstein Richard, Daniel Smith, Jordan Ambrose, Laurel Moody. 2012. “Headphone use and
pedestrian injury and death in the United States: 2004-2011”. Injury prevention 18(5): 287-290. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1136/injuryprev-2011-040161.
40.
Perra
Vasiliki-Maria, Socrates Basbas. 2019. “Distracted Walking and the Impact of
Mobile Phone use: A Literature Review”. Young Researches Seminar:
Thessaloniki, Greece. DOI:
https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.25825.15206.
41. Byington Katherine, Dawid Schwebel. 2013. “Effects of
mobile Internet use on college student pedestrian injury risk”, Accident
Analysis & Prevention 51: 78-83. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.11.001.
42.
Griffiths
Jeffrey, John Hunt, Martin Mathew. 1984. “Delays at Pedestrian Crossings: Site
Observations and the Interpretation of Data”. Traffic
Engineering and Control 25:
365-371.
43.
Kim
Dohyung. 2019. “The transportation safety of elderly pedestrians: Modeling
contributing factors to elderly pedestrian collisions”. Accident
Analysis & Prevention 131:
268-274. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2019.07.009.
44.
Govinda Lalam, Dodappaneni Abhigna, Parvathy Nair, Ravi Shankar.
2020. “Comparative study of pedestrian crossing behaviour at uncontrolled
intersection and midblock locations”. Transportation Research Procedia 48: 698-706. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trpro.2020.08.070.
45.
Distefano Natalia, Salvatore Leonardi, Giulia Pulvirenti.
2022. “Analysis of pedestrian crossing behaviour at roundabout”. Transportation Research Procedia 60: 28-35. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trpro.2021.12.005.
46. Kim Teo-Ho, Joe-Mu Won, Gi-Mok Bae, Kyung-Do Kim. 2006. „The development of pedestrian
signal timing models considering pedestrian behavior and location”. KSCE Journal of Civil Engineering 10: 131-136. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02823931.
47.
Ferenchak Nicholas. 2016. “Pedestrian age and gender in
relation to crossing behavior at midblock crossings in India”. Journal of Traffic and Transportation
Engineering 3(4): 345-351. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtte.2015.12.001.
48.
Wang
Huarong, Zhan Gao, Ting Shen, Fei Li, Jie Xu, David Schwebel. 2020. “Roles of
in-dividual differences and traffic environment factors on children’s
street-crossing behaviour in a VR environment”. Injury Prevention 26(5): 417-423. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1136/injuryprev-2019-043268.
49.
Macedo Marcia, Maria Maia, Emilia Rabbani, Oswaldo Neto, Mauricio Andrade. 2022. „Traffic accident prediction model for
rural highways in Pernambuco”. Case Studies on Transport Policy 10(1): 278-286. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cstp.2021.12.009.
50.
Tulu Getu, Simon Washington, Md. Mazharul Haque, Mark King.
2015. “Investigation of pedestrian crashes on two-way two-lane rural roads in
Ethiopia”. Accident Analysis & Prevention 78: 118-126. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2015.02.011.
51.
Sheykhfard Abbas, Farshidreza Haghighi, Trond Nordfjærn,
Mostafa Soltaninejad. 2020. “Structural equation modelling of potential risk
factors for pedestrian accidents in rural and urban roads”. International journal of injury control
and safety promotion 28(1): 46-57. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/17457300.2020.1835991.
52.
Haghighi Morteza, Homayoun Sadeghi-Bazargani, Fatemeh Aghdam,
Leila Jahangiry, Haidar Nadrian. 2021. “Gender Differences in Pedestrians’
Traffic Behaviors in Iran: A Study from a Candidate Safe Community”. Health Scope 10(1): e110886. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5812/jhealthscope.110886.
53.
Olakulehin
Olawale, Samuel Olowookere, Adeleke Abiodun, Emmanuel Folami, Josep Omole, Olusola Akanbi, Olamide Asifat, Damilda Akinloye,
Tobiloba Oyeyemi, Ayooluwa Olajubu, Olawookere Sa. 2019. “Knowledge, attitude,
practices of road traffic regulations among pedestrians in a university
community in Southwestern Nigeria”. European International Journal of
Science and Technology 8(3): 41-56. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4103/jehp.jehp_442_20.
54.
Jothula Yadav, Dumpeti Sreeharshika. 2021. “Knowledge,
attitude, and practice toward road safety regulations among college students in
Telangana state”. Journal of Education and Health Promotion
10(1): 10-25. DOI:
https://doi.org/doi:
10.4103/jehp.jehp_442_20.
Received 23.11.2024; accepted in revised form 15.03.2025
Scientific Journal of Silesian
University of Technology. Series Transport is licensed under a Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License
[1]
Department of Logistics, Warsaw University of Life Sciences, Nowoursynowska 166 Street, 02-787 Warszawa, Poland. Email: teresa_gadek-hawlena@sggw.edu.pl.
ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4350-1246
[2]
Department of Logistics, Warsaw University of Life Sciences, Nowoursynowska 166 Street, 02-787 Warszawa, Poland, Poland.
Email: konrad_michalski@sggw.edu.pl.
ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6997-352X